Improving public engagement with and understanding of science through a zoo-based primate research facility
Submitting Institution
University of PortsmouthUnit of Assessment
Psychology, Psychiatry and NeuroscienceSummary Impact Type
SocietalResearch Subject Area(s)
Medical and Health Sciences: Neurosciences
Psychology and Cognitive Sciences: Psychology
Summary of the impact
We have established a primate research centre (The Macaque Study Centre)
in a zoo environment (Marwell Zoo) for research into primate social
cognition. Visitors can watch the science taking place, which 1)
significantly increases their perception of the zoo as a place of
learning, 2) increases their knowledge about the specific research being
conducted, and 3) improves children's attitudes to science as an exciting
subject. Marwell Zoo integrate the research into their educational
workshops, and similar facilities are now being established in other zoos
in light of the demonstrable scientific, public engagement/involvement and
animal welfare benefits.
Underpinning research
The underpinning research summarised here was carried out under the
leadership of Dr Bridget Waller (Reader in Evolutionary Psychology,
University of Portsmouth, during the period 2006-2013) who established the
Macaque Study Centre (MSC) in 2011. Key academic co-investigators include:
Professor Kim Bard (1999 to present) and Dr Jerome Micheletta (Lecturer
from 2012).
Comparative cognitive research is crucial to elucidate the evolutionary
origins of human communication. Waller and Bard conducted seminal studies
comparing human facial expression with chimpanzee facial expression, and
found that the anatomical basis of similar expressions is almost identical
[1], providing strong support for the argument that human facial
expressions derive from a common primate ancestor. Waller went on to
develop standardised, anatomically informed stimuli to conduct comparative
cognitive experiments with chimpanzees, demonstrating that human face
processing is rooted in ancestral systems [2]. Waller has also used an
evolutionary approach to develop a novel explanation for the enormous
variation found in human facial muscles — that muscles necessary for basic
facial expressions are universal, but muscles underlying more
idiosyncratic movements are not [3]. Most recently, Waller collaborated
with scientists who study other modalities (vocalisations and gesture) to
develop a new theory about the importance of multimodality in the
evolution of human language [4].
The MSC is a new facility where research is taking place with public
involvement from the outset: even very recent findings have had an
educational impact on the public as they have witnessed the process from
data collection right through to the first dissemination of findings (via
educational materials and talks at the zoo). Research has been supported
through basic sciences funding (grants 1 and 2) but also through public
engagement with science schemes given the potential for public
participation during data collection (grant 3). The MSC is the first
facility for cognitive research with crested macaques (Macaca nigra)
in the world. The vast majority of comparative cognition research takes
place in laboratory environments, but our facility is situated in a
socially-housed zoo group and all research takes place on public view. The
animals voluntarily enter an individual testing room and work with a
researcher on cognitive tasks via a computerised touchscreen. The first
data resulting from the MSC was published in 2012, which demonstrated that
crested macaques follow the gaze direction of their conspecifics - a key
developmental stepping stone toward complex traits such as theory of mind
[5]. Interestingly, the macaques followed the gaze of their friends
(individuals with which they have long-term social bonds) more quickly
than non-friends, suggesting that the way in which primates communicate is
not automated and reflexive, but instead responsive to the specific
relationship between individuals. We conducted a similar experiment in the
wild showing that crested macaques respond more to the recruitment alarm
calls of their friends than non-friends [6] and may be more likely to
assist friends in chasing off a predator. Both findings add to the growing
body of evidence that aspects of complex human communication can be traced
back to our primate ancestors.
References to the research
1. Waller, B.M., Vick, S.J., Parr, L.A., Bard, K.A., Smith Pasqualini,
M.C., Gothard, K. & Fuglevand, A. (2006). Intramuscular stimulation of
facial muscles in humans and chimpanzees: Duchenne revisited. Emotion,
6(3), 367-382. DOI: 10.1038/1528-3542.6.3.367
(IF = 3.3, cited 27 times). This study was the first to compare the
functional morphology of facial muscles between humans and another
primate, and set the scene for the development of ChimpFACS (a systematic
anatomically based coding scheme to compare chimpanzee and human facial
expressions). Following this example, similar systems have now been
developed for use with many other animal species.
2. Parr, L.A., Waller, B.M. & Heintz, M. (2008). Facial
expression categorization by chimpanzees using standardized stimuli. Emotion,
8(2), 216-231. DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.216
(IF = 3.3, cited 18 times, top 25% for citations in subject field
psychology). This study was the first to use standardised stimuli
(developed using ChimpFACS) in facial expression categorisation tasks by
chimpanzees, which allowed us to probe the mechanisms underlying
chimpanzee's face processing skills in hitherto unavailable detail. The
findings showed that, like humans, chimpanzees rely on both featural and
configural cues to discriminate between conspecific facial expressions.
REF 2 output: 4-BW-002
3. Waller, B.M., Cray, J.J. & Burrows, A.M. (2008). Selection for
universal facial emotion. Emotion, 8(3), 435-439. DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.435
(IF = 3.3, cited 23 times, top 10% for citations in subject field
psychology). Here, we conducted dissections to explore the individual
variation present in human facial muscles. Muscle necessary for universal
facial expressions did not exhibit the same variation as other facial
muscles, which helps explain a long debated conflict between the
psychological and anatomical literature - that humans have different
facial muscles but can still produce universal facial expressions. REF 2
output: 4-BW-001
4. Slocombe, K.E., Waller, B.M. & Liebal, K. (2011). The language
void: The need for multimodality in primate communication research. Animal
Behaviour, 81(5), 919-924. DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.02.002
(IF = 3.5, cited 16 times, top 1% for citations in subject field
psychology). This study was the first to quantify how methods vary in
primate communication research depending on the modality under study, and
that theories of language evolution are flawed as a result. We propose a
multimodal approach to primate communication and language evolution
instead. REF 2 output: 4-BW-004
5. Micheletta, J. & Waller, B.M. (2012). Friendship affects gaze
following in a tolerant species of macaque (Macaca nigra). Animal
Behaviour, 83(2), 459-467. DOI:
10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.11.018
(IF = 3.1, cited 3 times). This study was the first to demonstrate that
the strength of social bond affects the speed at which primates follow one
another's gaze direction, a useful cue to important things in the
environment. The data contribute to the growing body of evidence that
`friendship' is a genuine concept in animal societies, and likely had an
important impact in the evolution of primate societies.
6. Micheletta, J., Waller, B.M., Panggur, M.R., Neumann, C., Dubosq, J.,
Agil, M. & Engelhardt, A. (2012). Social bonds affect anti-predator
behaviour in a tolerant species of macaque, Macaca nigra. Proceedings
of the Royal Society: B Series. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2012.1470
(IF = 5.7, cited 3 times). Here, we show that macaques respond more
strongly to recruitment alarm calls when they are produced by their
`friends', suggesting that friendship can provide a significant survival
advantage (even in life-threatening social contexts).
Grants sponsoring the research:
1. The Leverhulme Trust, Research Interchange Grant entitled "Chimpanzee
Emotions: Development of a facial action coding system" to Kim Bard, Lisa
Parr and Marcia Smith Pasqualini 2002-2005, (£128,031)
2. Leakey Foundation Research Grant `Adaptive function of facial displays
in crested macaques (Macaca nigra)' to Bridget Waller and Jerome
Micheletta, 2013-2014, $20,884
3. British Psychological Society Public Engagement Grant `Development and
evaluation of interactive exhibits promoting comparative psychology in a
zoo environment' to Katie Slocombe and Bridget Waller, 2012-2013, £19,340
Details of the impact
The MSC is a collaborative project with Marwell Wildlife (the charity
managing Marwell Zoo) who are committed to the educational and scientific
outcomes of the project [1] and advertise the project via their website
[6]. Marwell Zoo receives approximately 500000 visits per annum, of which
approximately 36000 visit the MSC. Visitors are from a broad cross-section
of socio-economic backgrounds and ages (approximately 10% under 21yrs old
[7]) many coming from groups difficult to engage in science communication
through other means. Visitors can watch the scientists working with the
macaques through a viewing window, and engage with information materials
surrounding the MSC (developed with Marwell education team). The materials
explain specific research findings plus background information. For
example, there is an interactive facial expression sign that invites
visitors to guess the `meaning' of each facial expression (relating
directly to our comparative facial expression research). The Marwell
education team integrate the research into their educational workshops
through interactive tasks based on our experiments with the macaques [2].
Public understanding and engagement has improved as a result of our
research. We assessed visitor knowledge and attitudes and visitor
behaviour systematically. We compared visitors on days when the scientist
was present, with visitors on days when the scientist was not present, and
also compared those who read the signs with those who did not. First,
visitors perceived greater learning when the scientist was present,
suggesting that the presence of the scientist affected how much people
felt they could (and did) learn from the experience. Second, visitors
exhibited more positive attitudes to science and conservation when the
scientist was present, suggesting that the scientist facilitated
attitudinal change. Finally, the visitors were tested on their
understanding of our specific research findings relating to facial
expression (present on the signs). Those visitors who had read the signs
exhibited greater knowledge and understanding than those who had not.
These are the first quantitative data to demonstrate actual, tangible
learning from a zoo-housed research focussed exhibit, and have since been
published in a high impact peer reviewed journal [3].
The research has had a specific educational impact on children. We
conducted a focus group with local college teachers, conducted
quantitative surveys with parents/guardians at Marwell Zoo and conducted
an online survey with teachers taking school groups to Marwell Zoo between
January and August 2013. Over 80% of respondents reported that the MSC
supported student learning at school and teachers rated `watching the
scientist' as the most educational activity at the MSC [8]. The potential
for further public engagement at this specific site has also been
recognised by the British Psychological Society, who awarded £19,340 to
Bridget Waller (and Katie Slocombe at the University of York) as part of
their public engagement grant scheme (see grant 3). Interactive
touchscreen games have been developed and installed at the MSC and
Edinburgh Zoo Budongo Trail chimpanzee enclosure based on the research
conducted at the two sites [9]. Overall, 24% of visitors (and 44% of
11-21yr olds) use the touchscreens. Thus, since installation in March
2013, we estimate 96,000 visitors have used the games in Edinburgh Zoo,
and 60,000 in Marwell Zoo. Visitors who engage with the games perceive
science as significantly more interesting than a control group, and
demonstrate increased knowledge about research findings explained in the
games [10]. As young people play on the games significantly more often
than adults, the games have the greatest impact within this age group.
Animal welfare has been enhanced by our research. The testing
process has a beneficial effect on the macaques by decreasing aggression
and increasing positive social behaviours [data published in a scientific
article: 4]. As the macaques voluntarily separate from the social group
when they use the touchscreens, this emulates wild patterns of behaviour
where the group members have brief episodes of separation followed by
reunion.
Practitioners have used our research findings. Within the zoo
community the Macaque Study Centre has attracted considerable interest.
Micheletta was invited to talk at the British and Irish Institute of Zoos
and Aquaria (BIAZA) Mammal Taxon Working Group (October 2012) to introduce
touchscreen training primates in a zoo environment. Dr. Antje Engelhardt
(German Primate Centre, Germany) is building a similar facility at
Darmstadt Zoo (Germany) modelled on the MSC (after visiting our facility
and seeing the benefits for public engagement, animal welfare and
scientific productivity [5]), and the Owl and Monkey Haven (Isle of Wight)
are working with us to develop a similar facility with two new species of
macaque.
Sources to corroborate the impact
- Marwell Wildlife: Letter of support from Chief Executive Officer
(Marwell Wildlife) detailing how our research collaboration fits with
their scientific and educational mission.
- Marwell Wildlife: Letter of support from Acting Head of Education
(Marwell Wildlife) detailing current practice (and future plans)
integrating our research into teaching workshops and educational
materials at Marwell Zoo.
- Published peer-reviewed scientific paper evidencing the educational
impact of our research on visitors at Marwell Zoo: Waller, B.M., Peirce,
K., Mitchell, H., Micheletta, J. (2012). Evidence of public engagement
with science: Visitor learning at a zoo-housed primate research centre.
PLOS ONE 7(9): e44680. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044680
(1683 article views since publication, above average for psychology
articles in PLOS ONE)
- Published peer-reviewed scientific paper evidencing the animal welfare
benefit of cognitive testing with zoo-housed primates: Whitehouse, J.,
Micheletta, J., Powell, L.E., Bordier, C., Waller, B.M. (2013). The
impact of cognitive testing on the welfare of group housed primates. PLOS
ONE. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078308
- Letter of support from Head of `Primate Sexual Selection' Research
Group at the Leibniz Institute for Primate Research (Germany) detailing
the development of a similar facility at Darmstadt Zoo (Germany) in
light of the published public engagement, animal welfare and scientific
impact of the MSC.
- Marwell Wildlife: Zoo website with details about the collaborative
project and links to the Macaque Study Centre website - http://www.marwell.org.uk/conservation/default.asp?css=1
- Marwell Wildlife: Trustees' Report and financial statements (2011)
detailing the number and profile of visitors to the zoo during the
period 2010-2011- http://www.marwell.org.uk/downloads/MarwellWildlifeStatutoryAccounts31Dec2011FINAL.pdf
- Report submitted to Marwell Wildlife detailing the results of our
qualitative and quantitative survey of parents and educational
professionals about the educational impact of the MSC.
- British Psychological Society: Link to British Psychological Society
website giving information about the educational games they funded us to
develop and evaluate at Marwell Zoo: http://www.bps.org.uk/news/are-you-cleverest-primate
- End of grant report submitted to the British Psychological Society:
summarises the findings of our evaluation of the educational impact of
the visitor games (that there is a specific and positive educational
impact on children and young people).