Changing Attitudes to Children’s Text Messaging and Literacy
Submitting Institution
Coventry UniversityUnit of Assessment
Psychology, Psychiatry and NeuroscienceSummary Impact Type
SocietalResearch Subject Area(s)
Medical and Health Sciences: Public Health and Health Services
Education: Specialist Studies In Education
Psychology and Cognitive Sciences: Psychology
Summary of the impact
Wood and Plester conducted the first empirical research into the
impact of text messaging on
children's literacy abilities, the results of which have impacted on:
- Public attitudes, by challenging media accounts of the
alleged detrimental impact of texting on
children's understanding of standard English;
- Public policy on literacy in England by informing Department
for Education and National
Literacy Trust reports and research regarding the value of informal
language use to written
language skills;
- Practitioners in the UK and worldwide, who now use text
language as a way of teaching
English to young people.
Underpinning research
Wood and Plester led research internationally into the
relationship between text messaging and
children's literacy development. They were the first to demonstrate
empirically that the associations
between textism use (use of text abbreviations / alternative spellings)
and literacy outcomes were
positive, and that textism use may contribute causally to children's
spelling development over time.
The team were also the first to investigate the educational impact of
giving 9-10-year-old children
mobile phones for text messaging. Their research revealed no evidence of
detrimental impact, and
some evidence of positive contribution to spelling outcomes. They also
found that children with
dyslexia appear to avoid using the textism types that best support
literacy development. These
studies have been cited as the basis for other work that has been
conducted internationally and
which has replicated and extended these findings (e.g. Coe & Oakhill,
2011; Kemp and Bushnell,
2011; Durkin, Conti-Ramsden & Walker, 2011; Powell & Dixon, 2011;
Drouin, 2011; Grace, et al,
2012). Prior to the publication of these papers, interest in text-speak
was limited to linguistic
analyses by Thurlow (2006) and Crystal (2008), who argued against the
media portrayal of texting
as problematic for children's literacy. However there was no published
empirical test of this claim.
In 2005-6, Plester (Senior Lecturer at Coventry until retirement in 2009
then honorary research
fellow) and Wood (Reader then Professor at Coventry throughout)
conducted exploratory work to
examine whether there was any evidence of an association between text
messaging behaviour
and academic outcomes. They found that children (N=65) who were more
frequent texters tended
to do less well on tests of cognitive ability than children who texted
less frequently. However,
children who used more textisms during a text translation task tended to
have better verbal
reasoning scores. Data was then collected on a further sample of children
(N=35) who completed a
spelling task and another text translation task. It was found that the
children who used the most
textisms were the children who tended to score most highly on the test of
standard spelling ability
[1]. Although small-scale and exploratory, this was the very first paper
published on this topic.
Literacy is a practitioner journal which is read by members of the
United Kingdom Literacy
Association, mainly teachers and teacher educators. Wood and
Plester's paper is the single most
downloaded (6,045 downloads between 2008 and 2011) and cited paper in the
journal's history,
and was shortlisted for the UKLA Literacy Publication Award in 2009.
This was followed up by a larger scale study (N=88) in 2006-7
investigating the relationships
between children's use of textisms in a simulated text messaging task and
variables such as verbal
reasoning ability, spelling, reading and phonological awareness. It was
hypothesised that
phonological awareness was the variable mediating the relationship between
textism usage and
literacy scores, as this skill underpins literacy and most textisms are
phonetic spellings. The
research found that there was significant shared variance between
phonological awareness and
textism use, but also found that textism use could still explain the
unique variance in reading ability
after controlling for individual differences in a wide range of other
contributing variables [2]. This is
the single most cited paper published in the British Journal of
Developmental Psychology during
the REF census period, attracting almost twice as many citations as the
second most cited paper.
Subsequently, the British Academy funded Wood to conduct a
longitudinal study (2007-9) to
investigate whether the patterns observed were indicative of causal
relationships over one
academic year. This study (N=119) was both the first substantial
longitudinal study to examine the
impact of textism use on the development of children's reading and
spelling over time, and the first
to use samples of children's actual text messages, rather than tasks that
simulated text messaging
behaviour. It was also the first to determine the direction of causality
in the previously reported
positive relationships between textism use and literacy outcomes. Once
again the research found
that children's textism use could predict unique variance in their
spelling development over time,
but spelling and reading development could not predict textism use,
indicating a unidirectional
causal relationship [3]. Given these results, a related research question
was explored, to determine
whether children with dyslexia displayed the same pattern of results.
Additional data collected in
2008-9 showed that children with dyslexia appeared to use fewer phonetic
textisms than their
typically developing peers, and that the observed relationships between
literacy variables and
textism use were not observed in the sample with dyslexia [4].
Subsequently, Wood conducted the first intervention study to
examine the impact of giving mobile
phones to primary school children as a form of literacy intervention. Wood
led a Becta-funded
randomised control study in 2010 (N=114), giving mobile phones to 9-10
year-old children to use
every weekend over a 10-week period. The research collated detailed
information on the children's
phone usage, monitoring the numbers of messages sent and received, as well
as textism use. This
study found no evidence of a significant benefit of text messaging over
this period when compared
to that of the control group. However, when the intervention group's data
was analysed
longitudinally, the contribution of textism use to spelling development
found previously [3] was also
found in this sample [5].
References to the research
[1] Plester, B., Wood, C., & Bell, V. (2008). Txt msg n
school literacy: does texting and
knowledge of text abbreviations adversely affect children's literacy
attainment? Literacy, 42
(3), 137-144 (IF: 0.294; Citations: 36)
[2] Plester, B., Wood, C. & Joshi, P. (2009). Exploring the
relationship between children's
knowledge of text message abbreviations and school literacy outcomes. British
Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 27 (1), 145-161 (IF: 1.33; Citations: 36)
[3] Wood, C., Meachem, S. Bowyer, S., Jackson, E.
Tarczynski-Bowles, M. L., & Plester, B.
(2011). A longitudinal study of children's text messaging and literacy
development. British
Journal of Psychology, 102 (3), 431-442 (IF: 2.103; Citations: 3)
[4] Veater, H.M., Plester, B., & Wood, C. (2011). Use of text
message abbreviations and literacy
skills in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia, 17(1), 65-71
(IF: 1.227; Citations: 3)
[5] Wood, C., Jackson, E., Hart., L., Plester, B., & Wilde,
L. (2011) The effect of text messaging
on 9- and 10-year-old children's reading, spelling and phonological
processing skills, Journal
of Computer Assisted Learning, 27 (1), 28-36 (IF: 1.632;
Citations: 9)
Key funding
• Wood & Plester, £7439, British Academy, A cross-lagged
longitudinal study of children's use of
text message abbreviations and their relationship to reading and spelling
development. June
2007-December 2008. This project has been developed into a British Academy
Case Study
and is referred to in their funding brochure `Inspiring Excellence'.
• Wood £7,492, British Academy, Understanding the relationship
between texting and spelling
attainment. February 2010- August 2011.
• Wood & Plester, £19,993, Becta, Children's use of mobile
phone text messaging and its impact
on literacy development in primary school. March 2008-March 2009.
Details of the impact
Wood and Plester's empirical research has not only challenged the
conventional tabloid wisdom
that textism use is detrimental to the reading ability of children, it has
initiated a new line of
empirical academic research and directly impacted on public policy and
practitioners' use of texting
language in their teaching.
Impact dissemination process and dates
The initial data [1] was published in a practitioner journal in order to
generate discussion amongst
teachers and within schools regarding how mobile phones were perceived by
educators. As a
result of this paper and the media debate stimulated, Wood was
seconded to Becta for a three-
month period from January 2010, to review the academic evidence of which
technologies improve
English skills in school children, which included the mobile phone
research. The Becta review is
available via the Institute of Education's Digital Resources Archive (http://dera.ioe.ac.uk/1670/).
In December 2012 Wood was invited to present a seminar at the
Department for Education on the
potential of technology to impact on children's literacy, which reviewed
the research evidence on e-
books and mobile phone use including Wood's own research. Wood
was then asked to review
and comment on the draft National Curriculum for English in Key Stages 1
and 2 with respect to its
treatment of technology. Wood's seminar was also attended by
OFSTED and Dyslexia Action, and
Wood was invited to discuss her work in more detail with these
organisations in 2013. Coventry
data [2] was referred to by Crystal in his book `Txting: the Gr8 Db8', and
the findings [2,3] were
also widely disseminated to the general public via a published case study
on the British Academy
website [a] and through mainstream national and international media
[b,c,d,e]. Becta then funded
the intervention study [5] to examine the question of whether mobile phone
ownership could boost
literacy attainment in primary school children. Most recently, Wood's
research [1,2,3,4,5] was
covered in the BBC4 documentary series `Growing Children' as part of the
programme on dyslexia,
screened in August 2012 (average daily reach for BBC4 in the week it was
screened: 2.3 million
viewers).
Following the media coverage of the research, Wood was invited to
be a keynote speaker at the
Youth Libraries Group (YLG) annual conference, with 200 delegates
from across the UK (2009).
The presentation included reviews of her research with respect to
technology and literacy, and
discussed the ways in which technology is best integrated with the
curriculum. The Youth Libraries
Group presentation was published as a book chapter in `Read to
Succeed', a text aimed at
librarians who work with children and young people (Wood, C. 2011.
`How children begin to read'.
In: Court, J. (Ed.) Read to Succeed: Strategies to Engage Children and
Young People in Reading
for Pleasure (pp15-28). London: Facet. Sales figures: 670). Wood
was also invited to be a panel
member by the National Literacy Trust at the launch of their 2012 annual
survey results. The
survey found children are more likely to read via digital sources than
traditional print media, and
Wood's research was identified as an important contribution to the
debate about the potential of
technology to impact positively on children's literacy [f].
Beneficiaries Primary and secondary school teachers,
trainee teachers, youth and school
librarians, parents, school-age children.
Nature of the impact and evidence
The text messaging research has impacted on public attitudes in
relation to popular perceptions
and understandings of how mobile phones may influence the development of
young people's
written language skills. Prior to the publication of the research [1]
there was an accepted popular
media argument that text messaging was responsible for the apparent
decline in literacy
attainment amongst children. For example, in the Guardian http://tinyurl.com/oyf4hof
and famously,
in the Daily Mail http://tinyurl.com/y7g7lvc.
Wood's research examined these questions empirically,
and directly challenged the assumptions about the negative impact of
texting behaviour. As a direct
result of the Coventry research, online blogs and media coverage worldwide
now increasingly
challenges the idea of textism use being detrimental to young people's
literacy skills, citing the
Plester and Wood's research as evidence [for example: b,c,d,e,f].
The work has begun to impact on public policy within the Department
for Education (DfE).
Following the DfE seminar in December 2012, Wood's research [2]
was incorporated into a
Department for Education report on research evidence on writing [g]. The
report acknowledged
that there was now evidence that texting could make a positive
contribution to children's
phonological awareness. Wood's research has also impacted the work
of the National Literacy
Trust [f]. Director, Jonathan Douglas, states "We have used it as a
central evidence base for our
policy work with the Department for Education. It has stimulated further
research which the
National Literacy Trust itself has undertaken. It has also supported new
practices in teaching
literacy in the 900 National Literacy Trust schools, members of our
network who develop and share
innovative evidence-based approaches to literacy...It has helped us to
understand the contribution
that texting and informal digital communication makes to formal skills
associated with writing. We
have taken this exploration further with our own research which
demonstrates that blogging in
leisure time impacts positively on writing skills... It has helped us
understand the importance of
phonological awareness in developing writing skills and that this
awareness can be developed in a
playful and relevant way. This emphasises the importance of
contextualising literacy pedagogy in
real life interests and experiences. This has fed into programmes such
as the National literacy
Trust's Premier League Reading stars project which operates in 1,000
English schools."
In terms of impact on practitioners, there is evidence that
innovative teachers worldwide were
beginning to use `text-speak' to aid learning English prior to the
publication of Wood and Plester's
empirical research. For example, teachers were using it in Scotland and
the Scottish Qualifications
Authority (SQA) came under fire for allowing children to write answers in
text speak, as long as
they showed an understanding for the subject, although pupils who used
phrases such as "2b r nt
2b" or "i luv u" would not be able to get top marks' [h]. This example is
taken from an article about
Wood's research, which suggests even where Plester and Wood's
work may not have inspired the
use of texting in education, it may have contributed to legitimizing it.
In the US, the best selling
book `Teaching Generation Text: Using Cell Phones to Enhance Learning'
outlines stories from
educators, parents, students, counsellors and anyone using cell phones to
enhance learning -
suggesting it is currently taking place in the classroom [i]. The blog
associated with the book [j]
directly references Wood's research, and the value of text
messaging in teaching. It also outlines
how the research could be employed in the classroom such as to translate
poetry into `text-speak'
in order to aid understanding and create useful summaries.
The research is also endorsed by Marc Slater who developed Ultimate
Spelling software, a widely
used worldwide spelling software programme for students. He has commented
in a recent press
release, "This latest research is very interesting. For texting to help
kids spell, it sounds great. We
just hope that research continues to support these findings as texting
seems to be more and more
a part of everyday life." (http://www.prweb.com/releases/2012/10/prweb9955790.htm)
Conclusion
Wood and Plester's pioneering rigorous empirical research has
changed the way that the public,
policy makers and educators worldwide view, accept and use text-speak to
develop English
Language Skills in children and adolescents: "The full implications of
the research to policy makers
and practitioners have yet to be fully grasped. They are challenging
and, I believe, must lead to
questioning of how literacy is taught and assessed. And indeed what
literacy is. For this reason I
see this research as being at the very cutting edge of the global debate
which is defining what it
means to be literate in a society driven by, and delighting in, digital
communication" — Jonathan
Douglas, Director of the National Literacy Trust.
Sources to corroborate the impact
a. Wood, C., Plester, B., & Bowyer, S. (2009). Liter8 Lrnrs:
Is Txting Valuable or Vandalism?
British Academy Review, 14, 52-54. http://www.britac.ac.uk/news/news.cfm/newsid/14
b. Wood interviewed live on the Today Programme (BBC
Radio 4). 20th January, 2010. 8:38am.
Duration 5:11. No specific listener data for this programme are available
but Radio 4
programmes for that week reached 10,029,000 listeners, representing 20% of
individuals aged
over 15 years.
c. http://abcnews.go.com/Technology/txt-msgs-kidz-spell/story?id=12800449
d. http://blogs.wsj.com/digits/2010/01/21/texting-could-actually-help-kids-read-regular-english/
e. http://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=4099
This post is reported by the site owner to have
received a minimum of 50,000 views.
f. National Literacy Trust Impact Statement letter on Coventry University
research
g. http://www.education.gov.uk/schools/teachingandlearning/pedagogy/a00217040/research-evidence-on-writing
h. http://www.inthenews.co.uk/news/txting-good-for-kids-literacy-skills--$21385429.htm
i. Nielsen, L. and Webb, W. 2011. Teaching Generation Text: Using
Cell Phones to Enhance
Learning. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
j. http://teachinggenerationtext.blogspot.com/2012/04/texting-for-literacy.html